首页
外语
计算机
考研
公务员
职业资格
财经
工程
司法
医学
专升本
自考
实用职业技能
登录
外语
It is hard to conceive of a language without nouns or verbs. But that is just what Riau Indonesian is, according to David Gil, a
It is hard to conceive of a language without nouns or verbs. But that is just what Riau Indonesian is, according to David Gil, a
admin
2011-02-11
38
问题
It is hard to conceive of a language without nouns or verbs. But that is just what Riau Indonesian is, according to David Gil, a researcher at the Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, in Leipzig. Dr. Gil has been studying Riau for the past 12 years. Initially, he says, he struggled with the language, despite being fluent in standard Indonesian. However, a breakthrough came when he realized that what he had been thinking of as different parts of speech were, in fact, grammatically the same. For example, the phrase "the chicken is eating" translates into colloquial Riau as "ayam makan". Literally, this is "chicken eat". But the same pair of words also have meanings as diverse as "the chicken is making somebody eat", or "somebody is eating where the chicken is". There are, he says, no modifiers that distinguish the tenses of verbs. Nor are there modifiers for nouns that distinguish the definite from the indefinite. Indeed, there are no features in Riau Indonesian that distinguish nouns from verbs. These categories, he says, are imposed because the languages that western linguists are familiar with have them.
This sort of observation flies in the face of conventional wisdom about what language is. Most linguists are influenced by the work of Noam Chomsky--in particular, his theory of "deep grammar". According to Dr. Chomsky, people are born with a sort of linguistic template in their brains. This is a set of rules that allows children to learn a language quickly, but also imposes constraints and structure on what is learnt. Evidence in support of this theory includes the tendency of children to make systematic mistakes which indicate a tendency to impose rules on what turn out to be grammatical exceptions (e. g. "I dided it" instead of "I did it"). There is also the ability of the children of migrant workers to invent new languages known as creoles out of the grammatically incoherent pidgin spoken by their parents. Exactly what the deep grammar consists of is still not clear, but a basic distinction between nouns and verbs would probably be one of its minimum requirements.
Dr. Gil contends, however, that there is a risk of unconscious bias leading to the conclusion that a particular sort of grammar exists in an unfamiliar language. That is because it is easier for linguists to discover extra features in foreign languages--for example tones that change the meaning of words, which are common in Indonesian but do not exist in European languages--than to realize that elements which are taken for granted in a linguist’s native language may be absent from another. Despite the best intentions, he says, there is a tendency to fit languages into a mould. And since most linguists are westerners, that mould is usually an Indo-European language from the West.
It needs not, however, be a modern language. Dr. Gil’s point about bias is well illustrated by the history of the study of the world’s most widely spoken tongue. Many of the people who developed modern linguistics had had an education in Latin and Greek. As a consequence, English was often described until well into the 20th century as having six different noun cases, because Latin has six. Only relatively recently did grammarians begin a debate over noun cases in English. Some now contend that it does not have noun cases at all, others that it has two while still others maintain that there are three or four cases.
The difficulty is compounded if a linguist is not fluent in the language he is studying. The process of linguistic fieldwork is a painstaking one, fraught with pitfalls. Its mainstay is the use of "informants" who tell linguists, in interviews and on paper, about their language. Unfortunately, these informants tend to be better-educated than their fellows, and are often fluent in more than one language.
______ cannot serve as evidence of Noam Chomsky’s theory of "deep grammar".
选项
A、He never forgaved her for teasing him.
B、She beganed to feel a sense of panic.
C、Sheeps were grazing on the hillside.
D、There are a desk and two chairs here.
答案
D
解析
推断题。由题干中的Noam Chomsky定位至第二段。该段第五句指出;支持乔姆斯基理论的证据包括孩子们倾向于犯规律性的错误.他们倾向于在一些例外的情况下还应用某些语法规则,例如将“I did it”误写为“I dided it”。可见这里是说将一般浯法规则应用于特殊情况。[A]和[B]都是对过去时的错误应用, [C]是名词复数的错误应用,只有[D]不属于将某个一般语法规则应用于例外情况,故为答案。
转载请注明原文地址:https://jikaoti.com/ti/tQuYFFFM
0
专业英语八级
相关试题推荐
Ricci,45,isnowstrikingoutonperhapshisboldestventureyet.HeplanstomarketanEnglish-languageeditionofhiselegant
SomerecenthistorianshavearguedthatlifeintheBritishcoloniesinAmericafromapproximately1763to1789wasmarkedbyin
ThereisagreatconcerninEuropeandNorthAmericaaboutdecliningstandardsofliteracyinschools.InBritain,thefactthat
1Theword"environmentalist"hasbeenbandiedaboutsomuchthatapersoncan’tbesureinagiveninstancewhetherthewor
Assuburbsgrew,businessesmovedintothenewareas.Largeshoppingcentrescontainingagreatkindofstoreschangedcon-
Assuburbsgrew,businessesmovedintothenewareas.Largeshoppingcentrescontainingagreatkindofstoreschangedcon-
A、ScientistsattheKennedySpaceCenterareattemptingtogrowvegetablesthatcouldsurvive.B、Inthespecialcontainersplant
A、OnThursday.B、OnFriday.C、OnSaturday.D、OnSunday.B关于Mirspacestation的残骸坠落在太平洋上的时间,从“RemnantsofRussia’sMirspacestati
A、WindowsoperatingsystemB、agraphicaluserinterfaceC、theuseoficonsD、theuseofpersonalcomputerB
Ofallthecomponentsofagoodnight’ssleep,dreamsseemtobeleastwithinourcontrol.Indreams,awindowopensintoaworl
随机试题
有关乳腺摄影的叙述,不正确的是
关于队列研究,下列哪项是错误的
龋齿按病变程度的分类为
出现大便隐血试验阳性.其上消化道出血量至少达到的数量是
阳明头痛的“引经药”应首选()
房地产估价师执业资格注册有效期为3年,有效期满前2个月,房地产估价师应持《房地产估价师注册证书》到原注册管理机构申请重新办理注册手续。
国内生产总值的平减指数的计算公式是()。
下列情形中,根据会计准则规定应当重述比较期间财务报表的有()。
对违法犯罪分子的改造工作,是教育人、挽救人和防止重新犯罪的特殊预防工作。()
在VisualFoxPro中,假设教师表T(教师号,姓名,性别,职称,研究生导师)中,性别是C型字段,研究生导师是L型字段。若要查询"是研究生导师的女老师"信息,那么SQL语句"SELECT*FROMTWHERE"中的应是( )。
最新回复
(
0
)